Skip to main content

Promiscuous words

Abstract

Promiscuity is frequently used to describe animal mating behaviour, and especially to describe multiple mating by females. Yet this use of the term is incorrect, perhaps reflecting an erroneous adoption of common language to pique reader interest. We evaluated the patterns of use and misuse of the word ‘promiscuity’ in a representative journal of animal behaviour. This survey highlights how inappropriately the term is used, and how it can conceal critical features of animal mating strategies with intriguing evolutionary significance. Further analysis of the scientific impact of papers identified by the term promiscuous or polyandrous revealed that the former were cited less frequently. We argue that using promiscuity to describe animal mating strategies is anthropomorphic, inaccurate, and potentially misleading. Consistent with other biological disciplines, the word promiscuity should be used to describe indiscriminate mating behaviour only, and that polygyny and polyandry should be used to describe male and female mating frequency respectively.

Introduction

Promiscuity is frequently, but largely incorrectly used to describe animal mating behaviour, perhaps reflecting an erroneous adoption of common language to pique reader interest. According to The Oxford English Dictionary, promiscuous originally referred to repeated, indiscriminate actions: “That is without discrimination or method: confusedly mingled, indiscriminate (1605) … Of an agent or agency: making no distinctions: undiscriminating (1633) … casual, carelessly irregular (1837)” [1]. Promiscuity was used to describe human sexual activity in the 19th Century, the essence (and costs) of which are colourfully observed in George Sala’s bawdy pantomime Harlequin Prince Cherrytop (1879): “Better frig, howe'er the mind it shocks, than from promiscuous … [fornication] … catch the pox” [2].

The term ‘promiscuity’ sneaked into the lexicon of evolutionary biology last century, particularly to describe mating behaviour e.g. [36] and is now widely entrenched (a Web of Knowledge (Thomson Reuters) search for ‘promiscu*’, limited to the fields of ‘Evolutionary Biology’, ‘Zoology’, ‘Behavioural Sciences’, and ‘Ecology’ returned over 700 publications). It is currently typically, although not exclusively [7], applied to describe female multiple mating or polyandry – the latter taking precedent [8].

Science often borrows words from common language: very early uses of the word promiscuous referred to surgical procedures [9], the use of barbiturates [10] and landscape management [11], and more recently molecular biologists use promiscuous to describe certain enzymes [12], gene regulators [13] and receptors [14] as promiscuous, precisely due to their non-specific nature. The use of these terms as scientific jargon draws on the general meaning of the word to highlight indiscriminating processes. This contrasts with its use as a descriptor for multiple mating behaviour, because the implied indiscriminating mate selection process is broadly wrong.

Females are rarely promiscuous, in the general meaning noted in the Oxford English Dictionary: the overwhelming evidence from diverse taxa confirms Darwin’s suggestion [15] that females are typically circumspect about their mates [16], accruing a variety of benefits from their discriminate mating [17, 18], including with multiple partners [19]. In general, we expect females to remain choosy, irrespective of the number of mating partners, the exception being species in which there is cryptic female choice e.g. [20].

Promiscuous has been used as an umbrella term to include polyandry, polygyny, and polygynandry [21]. While it may be useful to use a single term to describe mating strategies in which males and females mate multiply (arguably, the modal animal mating behaviour), promiscuous is unhelpful because it conflates both the nature (discriminating or not) and frequency of mating. In contrast, the terms monogamy, polygyny, polyandry and polygynandry refer to frequency only. We highlight this issue by evaluating the patterns of use and misuse in the scientific literature of the word ‘promiscuity’ to describe female mating strategies.

Use and misuse of promiscuous

We investigated whether polyandrous females were simultaneously described as promiscuous and exhibiting discriminating mate choice in papers published in a representative journal, Animal Behaviour. Drawing on papers published in the period 2000–2010, we identified those that contained ‘promiscuous’ (and its associated derivations) in either the abstract or main text. For each paper, we asked to which sex the term was applied (male, female or both), and whether the term was applied in a species in which pre-copulatory female choice had been experimentally demonstrated (either in the article itself or other published papers), or whether the authors inferred or suggested its presence in that species. Female mate choice is typically understood to mean a mating preference for different kinds of males [7, 15, 16], and is inferred from experiments or field observations showing that females prefer males according to the degree of exaggeration of secondary sexual characteristics e.g. [1618]. We reduced the likelihood of misinterpretation of each paper by ensuring it was assessed independently by at least two readers. We confined our analysis to the term promiscuous because other descriptors of animal mating behaviour (such as polygynous, polyandrous and polygynandrous) do not make inferences about the nature of mating – whether either sex is discriminating or not – and thus are not at issue.

In total, 39 papers were evaluated (see Table 1). ‘Promiscuous’ was applied to females in 23 cases, males in 2, and in 14 cases the term was either applied to both sexes or the focal sex was ambiguous (significantly, such ambiguity is impossible with precise language, such as polyandry and polygyny). For papers in which ‘promiscuous’ was applied to females or both sexes (37 papers), female choice was demonstrated or suggested by the authors themselves in 18 instances, while in 15 cases there was no published evidence of female choice (four cases were excluded as the papers were theoretical reviews or meta-analyses). So, in over half of the instances, promiscuous is evidently used incorrectly, a proportion that is likely to be substantially underestimated: the absence of evidence of female choice in the remaining cases is not evidence that female preferences are absent.

Table 1 Details of papers published in the journal Animal Behaviour that make reference to promiscuity

Using promiscuity to titillate the reader?

Promiscuity as a term to describe animal mating behaviour is undoubtedly anthropomorphic, probably accounting for the frequency of its use, especially amongst the primate literature. The discipline does not tolerate other anthropomorphisms in biological science; for example, the term forced copulation is preferred over rape [22], and infanticide preferred over murder [23]. Promiscuity has pejorative and androcentric connotations [20] and is likely to be emotionally evocative [24], typically saved for the females of the species (Table 1): while polygynous males maximise their fitness by mating at the highest rate, females are described as promiscuous. Perhaps promiscuous is used in titles and abstracts precisely because it is titillating, the notion of indiscriminate mating tapping into latent social taboos.

We explored the potential motivation for and consequences of using the term promiscuous by evaluating the citation metrics for papers retrieved by searches in Web of Knowledge (Thomson Reuters). We selected 15 journals and conducted two searches for each journal, using the terms (i) promiscuous OR promiscuity, and (ii) polyandrous OR polyandry (summarised in Table 2). We make the comparison with polyandry only because our previous analysis indicated that, in the vast majority of cases, promiscuity is used to describe female mating frequency (Table 1). Polygyny is widely understood to mean, based on the Greek etymology, multiple mating by males[24] and thus refers to an entirely different behaviour. Roughly a third of the papers included in the sample were identified by the term promiscuous or promiscuity in the title, abstract or key words. While this proportion ranged from 20–50% between journals, it was not correlated with the journal’s Impact Factor (Figure 1). Nevertheless, the mean number of citations of ‘polyandry’ papers (34 ± 7) per journal was marginally greater than that of ‘promiscuity’ papers (26 ± 4; Wilcoxon Sign-Rank test: p = 0.07), and the single-publication h-index of ‘polyandry’ papers (16 ± 2) was significantly higher than that of ‘promiscuity’ papers (9 ± 2; Wilcoxon Sign-Rank test: p < 0.0001) (Table 2). It is not clear whether this reflects an author’s publishing strategy, or that literature searches typically use the term polyandry over promiscuity.

Figure 1
figure 1

The proportion of ‘promiscuity’ papers in a journal was not associated with its Impact Factor (2012 Journal Citation Reports, Thomson Reuters) (Spearman’s ρ = 0.03, p > 0.9).

Table 2 Characteristics of papers retrieved by the search term ‘promiscuity’ or ‘promiscuous’ and ‘polyandry’ and ‘polyandrous’ in 13 journals from 2000 to 31st July 2013

Conclusions

Arguments over definitions can be tedious, but a cavalier use of borrowed words is unhelpful. Our surveys reveal a tendency to describe female rather than male mating strategies as promiscuous, despite the inherent contradiction in meaning. There was no evidence that journals of different standing publish more or fewer papers that use the term promiscuous, but authors searching for papers using the term promiscuous will generally retrieve lower impact publications.

Promiscuity has become so firmly entrenched in the literature as a synonym for polyandry that its accuracy is no longer questioned. But indiscriminately describing multiple-mating strategies as promiscuous conceals critical features of intriguing evolutionary significance. Indeed, records of truly promiscuous mating strategies, in which females (or males) mated indiscriminately would be remarkable, and predicted, for example, when the costs of mate choice are exorbitant. Like other emotionally evocative terms used to describe sexual behavior [25], promiscuity can be replaced with polyandry, polygyny and polygynandry, as appropriate – descriptive terms that are silent about the nature of mating, and devoid of sociological, psychological and moralistic connotations. Convention is no justification for imprecision, as our survey revealed: without evidence of indiscriminate mating behaviour, ‘promiscuity’ in evolutionary biology should be left well alone.

References

  1. The Shorter Oxford English Dictionary. 1983, Oxford: Oxford University Press

  2. Sala GA: New and Gorgeous Pantomime Entitled Harlequin Prince Cherrytop, etc. 1905, Printed for private distribution

    Google Scholar 

  3. Birdsall DA, Nash D: Occurrence of promiscuity among females in natural populations of deer mice (Peromyscus-maniculatus). Can J Genet Cytol. 1972, 14: 721-

    Google Scholar 

  4. Kushlan JA: Promiscuous mating behavior in white-ibis. Wilson Bulletin. 1973, 85: 331-332.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Merritt RB, Wu BJ: On the Quantification of Promiscuity (Or ‘Promyscus’ Maniculatus?). Evolution. 1975, 29: 575-578. 10.2307/2407270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Harris MP: Promiscuity in the shag as shown by time-lapse photography. Bird Study. 1982, 29: 149-154. 10.1080/00063658209476750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Shuster SM, Wade MJ: Mating Systems and Strategies. 2003, New Jersey: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  8. Nabours RK: Polyandry in the grouse locust, Paratettix texanus Hancock, with notes on inheritance of acquired characters and telegony. Am Nat. 1927, 61: 531-538. 10.1086/280174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Bovvee JW: A warning against promiscuous uterine curettage. Surg Gynecol Obstet. 1920, 30: 618-620.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Hambourger WE: A study of the promiscuous use of the barbiturates – their use in suicides. JAMA. 1939, 112: 1340-1343. 10.1001/jama.1939.62800140010011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Pickford GD: The influence of continued heavy grazing and of promiscuous burning on spring-fall ranges in Utah. Ecology. 1932, 13: 159-171. 10.2307/1931066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Khersonsky O, Tawfik DS: Enzyme Promiscuity: A Mechanistic and Evolutionary Perspective. Annu Rev Plant Physiol Plant Mol Biol. 2010, 79: 471-505.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Raab JR, Kamakaka RT: Insulators and promoters: closer than we think. Nat Rev Genet. 2010, 11: 439-446. 10.1038/nrg2765.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Bird MK, Lawrence AJ: The promiscuous mGlu5 receptor - a range of partners for therapeutic possibilities?. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2009, 30: 617-623. 10.1016/j.tips.2009.09.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Darwin C: The Descent of Man, and Selection in relation to Sex. 1874, London: John Murray

    Book  Google Scholar 

  16. Andersson M: Sexual Selection. 1994, New Jersey: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  17. Kirkpatrick M: Good genes and direct selection in evolution of mating preferences. Evolution. 1996, 50: 2125-2140. 10.2307/2410684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Jennions MD, Petrie M: Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences. Biol Rev. 1997, 72: 283-327. 10.1017/S0006323196005014.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Arnqvist G, Nilsson T: The evolution of polyandry: multiple mating and female fitness in insects. Anim Behav. 2000, 60: 145-164. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1446.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Olsson M, Shine R, Madsen T, Gullberg A, Tegelström H: Sperm selection by females. Nature. 1996, 383: 585-

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Bertram SM, Gorelick R: Quantifying and comparing mating systems using normalized mutual entropy. Anim Behav. 2009, 77: 201-206. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Hilton DFJ: Is it really rape or forced copulation?. Bioscience. 1982, 32: 641-

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Packer C, Pusey AE: Adaptations of female lions to infanticide by incoming males. Am Nat. 1983, 121: 716-728. 10.1086/284097.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Emlen ST, Oring LW: Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science. 1977, 197: 215-223. 10.1126/science.327542.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Gowaty P: Sexual terms in sociobiology: emotionally evocative and, paradoxically, jargon. Anim Behav. 1982, 30: 630-631. 10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80079-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Malin Ah-King, Cordelia Fine, Root Gorelick and an anonymous referee for their insights. The Australian Research Council (DP0558265 to MAE and TMJ; DP0987360 to MAE; and DP110101163 to KBM) and the University of Melbourne (Career Interruption Research Fellowship to TMJ) support our research on the mating behavior of animals.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark A Elgar.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MAE, TMJ and KBM collated and prepared the data; MAE, TMJ and KBM analysed the data; MAE, TMJ and KBM drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Elgar, M.A., Jones, T.M. & McNamara, K.B. Promiscuous words. Front Zool 10, 66 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-10-66

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-10-66

Keywords