Skip to main content
  • Correspondence
  • Open access
  • Published:

The evolutionary causes of egg rejection in European thrushes (Turdusspp.): a reply to M. Soler

Abstract

No abstract.

Correspondence

A response to: Soler: No evidence of conspecific brood parasitism provoking egg rejection in thrushes. Frontiers in Zoology 2014, 11:68.

Several species of European thrushes (genus Turdus) reject foreign eggs, yet the evolutionary causes of this behavior remained unclear. Recently, we concluded that conspecific (CBP), rather than interspecific brood parasitism (IBP) is responsible for the patterns of foreign egg rejection in these taxa [1]. In his comment M. Soler does not agree with this conclusion. Here we address each of his four main points to demonstrate the principles that (i) a theory is never out of date but is either supported or discredited, and (ii) it is not the use of references, but instead the collection and analysis of new data, which advances scientific progress.

First, Soler argues that the costs of CPB are too low to select for the evolution of host defenses and that the foreign eggs are too similar to host eggs for effective discrimination. That costs of CPB seem to be generally low across various species does not necessarily mean that such costs are always low (see also p. 51 in [2]). For example, IBP is generally highly costly to hosts but in several hosts of the brown-headed cowbird (Molothrus ater), there is little or no detectable cost of IBP [3]. Regarding egg similarity, inter-clutch variation in both Turdus species that we studied is sufficiently high to allow for foreign conspecific egg discrimination both in theory [4] and in reality [1].

Second, Soler takes issue with the maintenance versus decay of egg rejection behaviors as an antiparasitic host defense in the absence of ongoing brood parasitism. Crucially, the empirical studies referenced by Soler reported that the rejection costs associated with the ejection of foreign eggs were essentially nil. In contrast, we documented non-negligible rejection errors and costs, even in response to foreign conspecific eggs. Therefore our study system does not fit the critical assumption of Soler’s argument which holds only for “a trait which does not decrease individual fitness”.

Third, Soler argues that aggression towards cuckoo dummies combined with the foster parents’ reluctance to feed cuckoo nestlings are a clear indication of past evolutionary exposure to IBP. This argument is flawed (see [5]). Song thrush (Turdus philomelos) show very low and nonspecific aggression towards cuckoo dummies and cuckoo chicks in experimental nests die not because of host chick discrimination but because the smooth nest-cup design prevents successful eviction of host nestmates by the hatchling cuckoo, forcing it into fatal competition with host chicks. In turn, blackbirds (T. merula) are aggressive not only to cuckoo dummies but to any intruders near their nests, including harmless pigeons (Columba livia), indicating that their aggression did not specifically evolve in response to IBP [6]. Cuckoo chicks cross-fostered to blackbird nests also do not survive to fledging but not because blackbirds were reluctant to feed cuckoo chicks, as the parasitic nestling’s mass typically increased with time [7]; instead, cuckoo chicks “had bloated bellies and suffered from diarrhea” implying that it is the types of food delivered by blackbirds that caused cuckoo deaths (p. 719 in [7]). In general, switching to new types of food is an unlikely defence against brood parasite chicks: it would be maladaptive for any hosts to evolve novel food habits as a defense against IBP because such change would not prevent most costs of IBP (see [8]).

Last, Soler pointed out that reported current rates of CBP in thrushes are too low to select for CBP discrimination, and should be much higher (e.g., estimated to be at least 22% in black-billed magpie Pica hudsonia: [9]). However, although their calculations included non-negligible discrimination errors and costs in that host species, its clutch size is 50–100% higher than those of thrushes (especially in New Zealand: [10]), rendering the cost of parasitism in magpies relatively low, and thus predicting the cost threshold for the evolution of CBP rejection relatively high compared to our taxa. Furthermore, as we explained [1], we severely underestimated parasitism rates in our study populations. Moreover, high current parasitism rates are not critical to provide support for CBP hypothesis; previously [11] supported CBP scenario based on current CBP rates of 2.7% in their study population of house sparrows (Passer domesticus).

Studies of IBP hosts routinely interpret high rejection of foreign eggs in the absence of current IBP as an evidence of historical IBP. We do not see any empirical or theoretical arguments that would prevent us from applying the identical logic to CBP systems. Just like in any other area of evolutionary biology, so in the study of IBP and CBP we should be able to infer past evolutionary causes events from present biological evidence. However, we agree with M. Soler that any conclusions based on such indirect evidence are essentially probabilistic. Accordingly, we can never reliably reconcile how important role CBP or IBP played in long past in virtually any species – including current suitable hosts of IBP which might have evolved their egg discrimination due to past CBP and not IBP [1]. However, the large-scale geographic and comparative approaches used by [5] and [1] demonstrate one way to overcome at least some of the limitations of inferring evolutionary forces responsible for the existence of current adaptations in organisms.

References

  1. Samas P, Hauber ME, Cassey P, Grim T: Host responses to interspecific brood parasitism: a by-product of adaptations to conspecific parasitism?. Front Zool. 2014, 11: 34-10.1186/1742-9994-11-34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Pirie M: How to Win Every Argument. 2006, Continuum International Publishing Group, New York

    Google Scholar 

  3. Hauber ME: Hatching asynchrony, nestling competition, and the cost of interspecific brood parasitism. Behav Ecol. 2003, 14: 224-235. 10.1093/beheco/14.2.227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Cassey P, Ewen JG, Blackburn TM, Hauber ME, Vorobyev M, Marshall NJ: Eggshell colour does not predict measures of maternal investment in eggs of Turdus thrushes. Naturwissenschaften. 2008, 95: 713-721. 10.1007/s00114-008-0376-x.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Grim T, Samaš P, Moskát C, Kleven O, Honza M, Moksnes A, R’skaft E, Stokke BG: Constraints on host choice: why do parasitic birds rarely exploit some common potential hosts?. J Anim Ecol. 2011, 80: 508-518. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2010.01798.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Grim T, Honza M: Differences in behaviour of closely related thrushes (Turdus philomelos and T. merula) to experimental parasitism by the common cuckoo Cuculus canorus. Biologia. 2001, 56: 549-556.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Grim T: Cuckoo growth performance in parasitized and unused hosts: not only host size matters. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006, 60: 716-723. 10.1007/s00265-006-0215-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Yang C, Stokke BG, Antonov A, Cai Y, Shi S, Moksnes A, R’skaft E, Møller AP, Liang W, Grim T: Host selection in parasitic birds: are open-cup nesting insectivorous passerines always suitable cuckoo hosts?. J Avian Biol. 2013, 44: 216-220. 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2013.00123.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Underwood TJ, Sealy SG, McLaren CM: Experiments on egg discrimination in two North American corvids: further evidence for retention of egg ejection. Can J Zool. 2004, 82: 1399-1407. 10.1139/z04-118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Samaš P, Grim T, Hauber ME, Cassey P, Weidinger K, Evans KL: Ecological predictors of reduced avian reproductive investment in the southern hemisphere. Ecography. 2013, 36: 809-818. 10.1111/j.1600-0587.2012.07927.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Soler M, Ruiz-Castellano C, Fernández-Pinos MC, R’sler A, Ontanilla J, Pérez-Contreras T: House sparrows selectively eject parasitic conspecific eggs and incur very low rejection costs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011, 65: 1997-2005. 10.1007/s00265-011-1209-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Our work is supported by the Human Frontier Science Program RGY69/2007 (to TG, PC & MEH) and RGY83/2012 (to MEH & TG).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tomas Grim.

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Samas, P., Hauber, M.E., Cassey, P. et al. The evolutionary causes of egg rejection in European thrushes (Turdusspp.): a reply to M. Soler. Front Zool 11, 72 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-014-0072-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-014-0072-y

Keywords