Skip to main content

Self-sacrifice in 'desperado' contests between relatives

Abstract

Intra-specific competition occurs in all animal species and can lead to escalated conflict. Overt fighting entails the risk of injury or death, and is usually avoided through the use of conventions or pre-fight assessments. However, overt fighting can be expected when value of the contest outweighs the value of the future, as contestants have little or nothing to lose. In these situations, respect for conventions and asymmetries between contestants can break down, and overt fighting becomes more likely (the desperado effect). Such conditions can arise in contests between queens over colony ownership in social insects, because the value of inheriting a colony of potentially thousands of helpers is huge and queens may have very limited alternative reproductive options. However, in social species the balance of possible outcomes may be influenced by inclusive fitness, as contestants are often relatives. Here we present a simple model based on social insects, which demonstrates that not fighting can be selectively advantageous when there is a risk posed by fighting to inclusive fitness, even when not fighting is likely to result in death. If contestants are related, a loser can still gain indirect fitness through the winner, whereas fighting introduces a risk that both queens will die and thereby obtain zero inclusive fitness. When relatedness is high and fighting poses a risk of all contestants dying, it can be advantageous to cede the contest and be killed, rather than risk everything by fighting.

Background

Intra-specific contests, primarily over resources and reproductive opportunities, are a ubiquitous aspect of animal behaviour. Such contests are highly significant as they can determine a large proportion (or indeed all) of an animal's lifetime fitness, and pose the risk of injury or death when they escalate to fights. Because of these risks, contest resolution is often achieved without fighting, through processes of mutual and/or self-assessment [1–3]. However, models of animal contest predict that escalated contests leading to fatal fighting become more likely when the outcome of a contest represents a greater proportion of lifetime fitness [1, 4]. That is, when contestants have little chance of fitness gains outside of the contest in question, fights should occur regardless of the risks involved because there is much to gain and little or nothing to lose [1, 5]. Furthermore, kinship between contestants will not prevent escalated contests as long as the ratio between the value of the future and the value of the contest is small [4, 5]. Fatal fighting can arise when Vo/V < 1 - r, where Vo and V are respectively the value of the future and of the current contest in terms of lifetime fitness, and r is the coefficient of relatedness between contestants [4]. Grafen [5] termed this the 'desperado effect', to describe the point where respect for conventions and asymmetries between contestants breaks down.

Escalated fighting occurs between relatives in a 'desperado' context in several circumstances: among male fig wasps and ants over mating opportunities [6–9], among nursery siblings over access to food resources in birds and mammals when young are overproduced [10, 11], and among social insect queens over resource inheritance [3, 12]. Contests are of utmost significance among queens of social insects as, in many species, colonies of potentially thousands of individuals are headed by one reproductive queen. Becoming queen thus represents a potentially huge fitness payoff, because once victorious, a queen is insulated from further environmental risks by the workers and can live for many years. Contests over the queen role can occur in normally monogynous species when several queens temporarily co-occur in one nest, such as during queen replacement, during colony reproduction via fission, and in pleometrotic foundress associations. In each case monogyny is soon restored: excess queens are culled by workers or killed during direct fighting between queens [12]. In such situations, there are usually very limited or no other reproductive opportunities for candidate queens. Thus, we would expect strong conflict between queens over colony inheritance to give rise to escalated fighting, and indeed this occurs in a range of species with variable degrees of relatedness between contestants (eg: honeybees [13, 14], bumblebees [15], ants [16–18] and wasps [19]).

In the ant Aphaenogaster senilis, contests over colony inheritance occur during emergency queen replacement following orphaning. A few full-sister replacement queens are produced and the first-born queen is usually behaviourally dominant over later emerging queens [20]. Late-born queens apparently cede to the first-born queen without fighting and are eventually killed by workers. In experimental groups with two queens, second-born queens did not fight and usually died, even when the first-born had one mandible ablated and could not bite effectively [21]. Alternative reproductive opportunities for late-born queens are almost non-existent, and thus ceding to the first-born is effectively a form of suicide [21]. This lack of reciprocal fighting apparently contradicts the predictions of contest models.

However, models based on hawk-dove games generally assume that in an encounter between two hawks, one will survive while the other is killed. This does not take into account the possibility that both contestants die and thus obtain zero inclusive fitness. In do-or-die contests such as those over colony inheritance, it is possible that the victor sustains a mortal injury during the fight, and subsequently dies or is killed by workers, leading to the death of both contestants [eg: [21]]. Furthermore, as replacement queens are reared from existing queen-laid eggs in most social hymenoptera (workers typically cannot produce female brood), further queens cannot be reared once existing brood are exhausted, and there is a risk of colony death through failure to requeen.

A queen that cedes and is killed can still gain indirect fitness though the victorious queen if they are related, while at the same time avoiding the risk of a zero fitness outcome. Thus, the risk of both contestants dying may impose a significant cost to fighting if relatedness between the contestants is high [21]. Highly related contestants in 'desperado' contests, such as late-born A. senilis queens, may secure more indirect fitness by not fighting than direct fitness from victory when the latter is devalued by the risk of both queens dying [21]. Here, we formalise this reasoning by developing a simple model to explain how not fighting can be selectively advantageous in a 'desperado' contest between relatives. We focus on 'desperado' situations, where contestant's alternative options for fitness gain are negligible or absent. The area of contests outside of this has been explored by a suite of reproductive skew models (reviewed in [22]). Our model was developed to explain behaviour observed in social insect colonies, and we initially focus on these systems. The applicability of the rationale to other taxa is explored in the discussion.

Life history of A. senilis

A. senilis is a monogynous, monandrous ant common to the Mediterranean basin, that reproduces by colony fission: queens are born with short wings and cannot fly, and only disperse with groups of workers on foot [23, 24]. Colonies consist of around 1,300 workers and are monodomous. Colonies temporarily contain multiple queens when new queens are reared during preparation for colony fission or when the queen has died and must be replaced. Because of haplodiploidy, these new queens are full-sisters and are related by 0.75. The frequency of requeening in the field is unknown, but orphaning in the lab results in the production of one to six replacement queens: in one experiment 30 orphaned colonies produced 2.0 ± 1.1 queens [20], and in a second experiment 60 orphaned groups produced 2.8 ± 1.3 queens each [21]. New queens are produced 14-17 days apart on average. Only one queen survives, and it is usually the first-born who inherits the colony while subsequently born queens are killed [20, 21].

Many strictly monogynous social insects go through transient polygynous phases: pleometrosis (founding of nests by multiple queens) occurs in many species as a regular mode of colony initiation [25], and fission occurs in a wide range of species, some of which are monogynous (eg: honeybees and army ants [26]). The frequency of queen replacement in nature is largely unknown, but it may be common in species that reproduce via fission (Cronin et al. unpublished data). Obligate monogyny in social insects is widespread, despite the fact that polygyny can be favoured for a variety of reasons including increased colony productivity, increased colony longevity, and high mortality of dispersing queens [[27], pp 126-134].

Findings

The model

We define our contest arena as a nest containing two queens and associated workers. This matches the experimental data on A. senilis, where the majority of contests during queen replacement are between two queens [20]. We assume monogyny and thus all but one queen will be killed. Reduction in queen number takes place via direct conflict between the queens if either queen initiates a contest, or by worker culling of one queen if neither queen initiates a fight. Queens cannot gain direct fitness by avoiding contest because queen selection will occur regardless of whether queens fight or not, and because queens have no reproductive opportunities outside the contest for colony ownership (thus Vo = 0; Table 1).

Table 1 Abbreviations and variables used in the model.

For simplicity, we consider the contest in isolation, without regard to strategies employed in the rest of the population. We assume that the winning queen gains equal fitness (V) whether a fight occurs or not, as the duration of conflict (a few days) is negligible compared to the length of time fitness is gained (a few years), and non lethal injuries sustained during a fight have no effect on reproductive value since the queens accrue resources indirectly through workers. The effect of relaxing these assumptions is discussed later. We further assume that workers select queens based on some criteria of asymmetry between queens, as is common in species where intra-specific contests arise (eg: order of arrival or age [20, 28, 29]). This asymmetry gives one competitor some advantage, and the queen which holds the advantage in the asymmetry (Q1) will win the contest if no fighting occurs (workers will kill the other queen). The advantage may be uncorrelated with the individual's intrinsic quality [30], and we consider it independently of fighting ability and reproductive value. In A. senilis, the asymmetry is determined by birth order, and the first born queen (Q1) generally inherits. How workers select one queen over the other is generally unclear, but may be related to a need to expedite the queen replacement process [20, 21], or because of queen manipulation [31, 32].

The possible contest outcomes are thus a) Q1 inherits the colony, either through killing Q2, or when Q2 is killed by workers in the case that no queen-queen fight occurs, b) Q2 inherits the colony by killing Q1, or c) both queens are killed fighting. Queens can gain direct fitness through inheriting the colony or indirect fitness when a relative inherits the colony. The fitness payoff for a given queen depends on the probability of winning the fight if they do fight, the risk of both queens dying, and the genetic relatedness between the queens.

Both queens may either choose to fight or refrain from fighting, and we assume a fight occurs if one queen initiates fighting. Q1 obtains a direct fitness of V if there is no fighting (workers kill Q2; see Table 1 for definitions of variables used in the model). When fighting occurs, Q1 has an inclusive fitness of:

( a V + ( 1 – a ) r V ) ( 1 – d )
(1)

Where a is the probability of Q1 winning an overt fight and 1 - a is the probability of Q2 winning. Genetic relatedness between contestants is denoted by r, and d represents the probability of a zero fitness outcome because both queens are killed in the contest.

From the above, it can be seen that Q1 prefers not to fight when:

V > ( a V + ( 1 – a ) r V ) ( 1 – d )
(2)

Q2 obtains no direct fitness if she elects not to fight (workers kill her), but she obtains an indirect fitness of r V. If Q2 fights, her inclusive fitness is (a r V + (1 - a) V) (1 - d). Thus, Q2 favours not fighting when:

r V > ( a r V + ( 1 – a ) V ) ( 1 – d )
(3)

Solving these inequalities yields the values for which not fighting is favoured by Q1 {2} and Q2 {3}. From {2} it is clear that Q1 never prefers fighting, which is intuitive as she stands to inherit if there is no fighting. For Q2, the decision to fight or not is independent of the value of the contest (V) but dependent on the probability of winning (a), relatedness (r) and the probability of a zero fitness outcome (d). Q2 prefers not to fight when:

d > ( 1 – r ) ( 1 – a ) / ( 1 – a + a r )
(4)

When queens are close relatives, have developed in the same environment, and have been fed equally, they may have an equal probabilities of winning a fight (i.e. a = 0.5), in which case {4} becomes d >(1 - r)/(1 + r). This relationship is depicted in Fig. 1 and indicates that when queens are full sisters (r = 0.75), as in A. senilis, Q2 fights only if the probability of both queens dying is low (d < 1/7 (= 0.143)).

Figure 1
figure 1

Conditions favouring not fighting by Q2 for contestants of equal fighting ability. Critical values of d for which not fighting (above the line) or fighting (below the line) is favoured, for all values of r, from equation {4}. When fighting abilities are equal (a = ½), the decision whether to fight or not is a function of d and r. This figure indicates that Q2 increasingly favours not fighting for increasing values of r and d. When contestants are full sisters (r = 0.75), Q2 will not fight unless the risk of both queens dying is very low (d < 0.143).

However, fighting asymmetries between contestants are likely if size differences exist or if differences in eclosion time results in development related fighting advantages. We explore this possibility by defining the probability of Q1 winning an overt fight as a linear function of Q1's relative fighting ability f, with a(f) = ½ (f + 1). Equation {4} then becomes: d >(1 - f)/[1 - f + 2r/(1 - r)] (Fig. 2). In this scenario, Q2 prefers not to fight when relatedness is high, and fighting asymmetry has little impact on this preference. However, when relatedness is low the effect of the fighting asymmetry is markedly stronger when it is in favour of Q1: being the better fighter only marginally increases Q2's propensity to fight whereas being the poorer fighter can dramatically reduce it (for low values of r). As it is likely that any asymmetry is in favour of the first-born [eg: [21, 33]], increasing fighting asymmetry can have the effect of reducing Q2's propensity to fight.

Figure 2
figure 2

Conditions favouring not fighting by Q2. Critical values of d for which not fighting (above the lines) or fighting (below the lines) is favoured by Q2 for various values of f, from equation {4}. Each line represents a value of relatedness (r = 0.1, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75 and 0.9, with r = 0.75 (full sisters) indicated by a thick line). For clone queens (r = 1), critical values are always 0, and not fighting is always favoured. Not fighting is favoured when relatedness is high or when Q1 is a better fighter.

Finally, it is worth considering that relative fighting ability affects not only the probability of winning the fight, but also the risk that both queens die: more evenly matched queens are more likely to have longer fights leading to a higher risk of both queens dying. In this case, the risk of both queens dying d becomes a diminishing function of increasing relative fighting ability. We can define d as a function of Q1's relative fighting ability f and a mortality index m, following d(f) = m e-αf². The variable m describes the lethality of fights in a given species while α defines the shape of the distribution (Table 1). Inequality {4} then becomes m >(1 - f)/[(1 - f + 2r/(1 - r))e-αf²]. Fig. 3 shows d(f) for α = 5, and the critical mortality above which Q2 does not fight. When queens have similar fighting abilities Q2 still prefers not to fight, but increasing disparity in fighting ability now increases Q2's propensity to fight, because the risk of a zero fitness outcome is reduced. Overall, Figs. 1 and 3 show that Q2 favours not fighting when there is a risk to her inclusive fitness: when the risk that both queens die from fighting is restricted to when queens have similar fighting abilities, the preference for not fighting by Q2 becomes similarly restricted. Thus, while the threat of a zero fitness outcome can favour not fighting by subordinate queens, large differences in fighting ability can negate this.

Figure 3
figure 3

Mortality risk curve for α = 5, and corresponding conditions favouring not fighting by Q2. Effect of relative fighting ability of Q1 (f) on diminishing the probability that overt fighting leads to the death of both contestants following d = m e-αf². The inset graph shows the distribution of the probability of both queens dying for m = 1 and α = 5. The risk of mortality of both queens is restricted to a smaller range of f values, and is close to zero for extremely biased fighting ability. The main figure shows that when the risk to inclusive fitness decreases with increasing asymmetry in fighting ability, Q2 is more likely to fight, particularly when it is a relatively strong fighter. Not fighting remains the preferred strategy when fighting represents a risk to inclusive fitness.

Discussion

Overt fighting can be expected in animal contests when the value of the contest is high relative to the value of the future [4] as contestants have nothing to lose. However, in contests between relatives there is something to lose, because fighting poses the risk of both contestants dying: in fighting with a related opponent, a losing individual can risk inclusive fitness loss by fatally injuring its competitor. Our results suggest that relatedness between contestants can lead to restraint under specific circumstances, even when not fighting means death and there are no alternative future reproductive opportunities. Our model indicates that this is possible when three conditions are met: i) contests are 'desperado' contests, with one winner and no alternative opportunities for fitness gains for the loser, ii) fighting poses a risk of mortality to the victor and, iii) contestants are highly related.

We have assumed that Q2 decides for herself how to act in a given situation. An alternative possibility is that her behaviour is manipulated by Q1 and/or workers, both of which benefit from Q1 inheriting if she is fit. It is unclear whether Q1 has the means of manipulating Q2 (eg: through chemical signals), but workers presumably do, as they control the amount of food Q2 receives as a larva, which may allow them to control her growth rate and hence delay her emergence, thereby advantaging Q1. Mechanisms of influencing offspring development and contest outcomes have also been described in vertebrates [33]. However, whether Q2 is manipulated or not in her decision to fight in A. senilis awaits investigation.

We employ the simplifying assumption of considering our contest in isolation, without regard to population-wide strategies. In reality, model parameters such as the value of the contest V are not independent of strategies employed by other colonies in the population, and this should be considered in a more comprehensive model. Furthermore, we assume a 2-queen contest arena and, while our model is applicable to most A. senilis colonies where only a few queens are produced, in some colonies Q1 may need to win multiple consecutive contests, and this may be the norm in other species. In such cases, an injury sustained in a fight will lower the relative fighting ability of the queen for the next fight. For instance, if Q1 wins the fight but is injured in the process she may have a lower fighting ability against a third queen. Nonetheless, unless Q3 is a markedly better fighter, she will still elect not to fight in order not to jeopardize her inclusive fitness (Fig. 3 with -0.25 < f <0). Conversely, if Q3 is a much stronger fighter (f < - 0.4) there is little risk that the queens will kill one another and Q3 will fight. However, in A. senilis late born queens do not fight even when Q1 has had one mandible ablated and cannot bite effectively (ie: f is close to -1) [21]. It may be that in this species, queens have not been selected to assess fighting abilities because queens are full-sisters and differ too little in fighting ability to ever make fighting a successful strategy for Q2 (Fig. 1).

Clearly, the conditions required for self sacrifice to occur are restrictive, and probably do not arise in most cases of animal contests because i) relatedness between contestants is insufficiently high or ii) there are other opportunities for fitness acquisition outside the focal contest. This includes contests among ant queens following pleometrosis: as foundresses are usually unrelated [25] overt fighting eventuates (eg: Lasius niger [16, 18], Messor pergandei [34], Azteca xanthacroa [35], Solenopsis wagneri (monogyne form) [12, 17]).

Conditions potentially giving rise to desperado contests between relatives can arise in several contexts in insects: among queens fighting over colony inheritance in monogynous species that can requeen or reproduce by colony fission (eg: A. senilis [20, 21] and honeybees [3], see also Table 2), and among wingless males of Cardiocondyla and Hypoponera ants, which may fight until only one remains in the colony and monopolises mating opportunities [6, 7, 36, 37]. However, the available evidence suggests overt fighting is usually the norm, possibly because relatedness between contestants is low as a result of polygyny and polyandry (Table 2).

Table 2 Examples of contests between relatives in a potential 'desperado' context in social insects.

Males also fight over females in many fig wasps [8, 9, 38], where the intensity of fights varies between species from causing no injury [9] to causing a high level of life-threatening injuries [38, 39]. Here, self-sacrifice is not expected because competing males are not highly related in many species [38] and males may avoid fights they are unlikely to win [39].

In vertebrates, siblings fight over food in some birds and mammals producing more young than can be reared [40]. However, in many cases these may not be true desperado contests as all young may be reared in exceptionally bountiful years, even in 'obligately' siblicidal species [11, 41]. Nonetheless, examples of overt fighting leading to siblicide exist for a range of avian species and some mammals [[41] and references therein], in which junior young are usually killed by senior siblings. Conditions favouring self-sacrifice may not arise because relatedness between vertebrate siblings (r = 0.5) is lower than for Hymenopteran full-sisters (r = 0.75) and, furthermore, development related asymmetries may be so extreme that the risk of contestants mortally injuring one another is negligible (ie: there is no risk to inclusive fitness), rendering self-sacrifice irrelevant. We predict that circumstances that can give rise to self-sacrifice are most likely to be found in monogynous and monandrous species of social insect that can requeen or that reproduce by colony fission, as relatedness is high and death of all but one contestant is inevitable. Stingless bees (Meliponini) fulfil these criteria, but queen-queen contests can be precluded by worker pre-emptively culling of queens [42] and details of queen-queen conflict are presently lacking. Closer investigation of contests in species where the specific conditions described arise could demonstrate further examples of self-sacrifice.

References

  1. Maynard-Smith JM, Price GR: Logic of Animal Conflict. Nature. 1973, 246: 15-18. 10.1038/246015a0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Parker GA: Assessment strategy and evolution of fighting behavior. J Theor Biol. 1974, 47: 223-243. 10.1016/0022-5193(74)90111-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Dietemann V, Zheng HQ, Hepburn C, Hepburn HR, Jin SH, Crewe RM, Radloff SE, Hu FL, Pirk CWW: Self assessment in insects: honeybee queens know their own strength. PLoS ONE. 2008, 3: e1412-10.1371/journal.pone.0001412.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Enquist M, Leimar O: The evolution of fatal fighting. Anim Behav. 1990, 39: 1-9. 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80721-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Grafen A: The logic of divisively asymmetric contests - respect for ownership and the desperado effect. Anim Behav. 1987, 35: 462-467. 10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80271-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Kinomura K, Yamauchi K: Fighting and mating behaviors of dimorphic males in the ant Cardiocondyla wroughtoni. J Ethol. 1987, 5: 75-81. 10.1007/BF02347897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Stuart RJ, Francoeur A, Loiselle R: Lethal fighting among dimorphic males of the ant, Cardiocondyla wroughtonii. Naturwissenschaften. 1987, 74: 548-549. 10.1007/BF00367076.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Hamilton WD: Wingless and fighting males in fig wasps and other insects. Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Insects. Edited by: Blum MS, Blum NA. 1979, New York: Academic Press, 167-220.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Greeff JM, van Noort S, Rasplus JY, Kjellberg F: Dispersal and fighting in male pollinating fig wasps. CR Biol. 2003, 326: 121-130. 10.1016/S1631-0691(03)00010-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Hofer H, East ML: Siblicide in Serengeti spotted hyenas: a long-term study of maternal input and cub survival. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2008, 62: 341-351. 10.1007/s00265-007-0421-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Drummond H, Rodriguez C, Vallarino A, Valderrabano C, Rogel G, Tobon E: Desperado siblings: uncontrollably aggressive junior chicks. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2003, 53: 287-296.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Balas MT: Conditions favoring queen execution in young social insect colonies. Insect Soc. 2005, 52: 77-83. 10.1007/s00040-004-0774-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Gilley DC: The behavior of honey bees (Apis mellifera ligustica) during queen duels. Ethology. 2001, 107: 601-622. 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00692.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Tarpy DR, Mayer MK: The effects of size and reproductive quality on the outcomes of duels between honey bee queens (Apis mellifera L.). Ethol Ecol Evol. 2009, 21: 147-153. 10.1080/08927014.2009.9522503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Cameron SA, Jost MC: Mediators of dominance and reproductive success among queens in the cyclically polygynous Neotropical bumble bee Bombus atratus Franklin. Insect Soc. 1998, 45: 135-149. 10.1007/s000400050075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Sommer K, Hölldobler B: Colony founding by queen association and determinants of reduction in queen number in the ant Lasius niger. Anim Behav. 1995, 50: 287-294. 10.1006/anbe.1995.0244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Bernasconi G, Keller L: Reproductive conflicts in cooperative associations of fire ant queens (Solenopsis invicta). Proc R Soc Lond B. 1996, 263: 509-513. 10.1098/rspb.1996.0077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Aron S, Steinhauer N, Fournier D: Influence of queen phenotype, investment and maternity apportionment on the outcome of fights in cooperative foundations of the ant Lasius niger. Anim Behav. 2009, 77: 1067-10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Nascimento FS, Tannure-Nascimento IC, Zucchi R: Behavioral mediators of cyclical oligogyny in the Amazonian swarm founding wasp Asteloeca ujhelyii (Vespidae, Polistinae, Epiponini). Insect Soc. 2004, 51: 17-23. 10.1007/s00040-003-0696-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Chéron B, Doums C, Fédérici P, Monnin T: Queen replacement in the monogynous ant Aphaenogaster senilis: supernumerary queens as life insurance. Anim Behav. 2009, 78: 1317-1325. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.08.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Cronin AL, Monnin T: Bourgeois queens and high stakes games in the ant Aphaenogaster senilis. Front Zool. 2009, 6: 24-10.1186/1742-9994-6-24.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Buston PM, Zink AG: Reproductive skew and the evolution of conflict resolution: a synthesis of transactional and tug-of-war models. Behav Ecol. 2009, 20: 672-684. 10.1093/beheco/arp050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Ledoux A: Un nouveau mode de bouturage de société chez la fourmi Aphaenogaster senilis Mayr. CR Acad Sci Ser D. 1971, 273: 83-85.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Boulay R, Hefetz A, Cerdá X, Devers S, Francke W, Twele R, Lenoir A: Production of sexuals in a fission-performing ant: dual effects of queen pheromones and colony size. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2007, 61: 1531-1541. 10.1007/s00265-007-0385-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Bernasconi G, Strassmann JE: Cooperation among unrelated individuals: the ant foundress case. Trends Ecol Evol. 1999, 14: 477-482. 10.1016/S0169-5347(99)01722-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Peeters C, Ito F: Colony dispersal and the evolution of queen morphology in social Hymenoptera. Annu Rev Entomol. 2001, 48: 601-630. 10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Crozier RH, Pamilo P: Evolution of Social Insect Colonies. 1996, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  28. Seppä P, Queller DC, Strassmann JE: Reproduction in foundress associations of the social wasp, Polistes carolina: conventions, competition, and skew. Behav Ecol. 2002, 13: 531-542. 10.1093/beheco/13.4.531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Bridge C, Field J: Queuing for dominance: gerontocracy and queue-jumping in the hover wasp Liostenogaster flavolineata. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2007, 61: 1253-1259. 10.1007/s00265-007-0355-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Maynard Smith J: Evolution and the Theory of Games. 1982, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  31. Monnin T, Ratnieks FLW, Jones GR, Beard R: Pretender punishment induced by chemical signalling in a queenless ant. Nature. 2002, 419: 61-65. 10.1038/nature00932.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Tarpy DR, Fletcher DJC: "Spraying" behavior during queen competition in honey bees. J Insect Behav. 2003, 16: 425-437. 10.1023/A:1024884211098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Drummond H, Rodriguez C, Schwabl H: Do mothers regulate facultative and obligate siblicide by differentially provisioning eggs with hormones?. Journal of Avian Biology. 2008, 39: 139-143. 10.1111/j.0908-8857.2008.04365.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Rissing SW, Pollock GB: Social-interaction among pleometrotic queens of Veromessor pergandei (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) during colony foundation. Anim Behav. 1986, 34: 226-233. 10.1016/0003-3472(86)90027-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Choe JC, Perlman DL: Social conflict and cooperation among founding queens in ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). The Evolution of Social Behavior in Insects and Arachnids. Edited by: Choe JC, Crespi BJ. 1997, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 392-406. full_text.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  36. Heinze J, Hölldobler B, Yamauchi K: Male competition in Cardiocondyla ants. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1998, 42: 239-246. 10.1007/s002650050435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Yamauchi K, Ishida Y, Hashim R, Heinze J: Queen-queen competition by precocious male production in multiqueen ant colonies. Curr Biol. 2006, 16: 2424-2427. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. West SA, Murray MG, Machado CA, Griffin AS, Herre EA: Testing Hamilton's rule with competition between relatives. Nature. 2001, 409: 510-513. 10.1038/35054057.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Moore JC, Obbard DJ, Reuter C, West SA, Cook JM: Fighting strategies in two species of fig wasp. Anim Behav. 2008, 76: 315-322. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Mock DW, Parker GA: The Evolution of Sibling Rivalry. 1997, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  41. Osorno JL, Drummond H: Is obligate siblicidal aggression food sensitive?. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2003, 54: 547-554. 10.1007/s00265-003-0667-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Wenseleers T, Hart AG, Ratnieks FLW, Quezada-Euan JJG: Queen execution and caste conflict in the stingless bee Melipona beecheii. Ethology. 2004, 110: 725-736. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.01008.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Pearcy M, Aron S, Doums C, Keller L: Conditional use of sex and parthenogenesis for worker and queen production in ants. Science. 2004, 306: 1780-1783. 10.1126/science.1105453.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Yamauchi K, Kimura Y, Corbara B, Kinomura K, Tsuji K: Dimorphic ergatoid males and their reproductive behavior in the ponerine ant Hypoponera bondroiti. Insect Soc. 1996, 43: 119-130. 10.1007/BF01242564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Paxton RJ, Weißschuh N, Engels W, Hartfelder K, Quezada-Euan JJG: Not only single mating in stingless bees. Naturwissenschaften. 1999, 86: 143-146. 10.1007/s001140050588.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Imperatriz-Fonseca VL, Zucchi R: Virgin queens in stingless bee (Apidae, Meliponinae) colonies: a review. Apidologie. 1995, 26: 231-244. 10.1051/apido:19950305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Nicolas Loeuille, Mathieu Molet, Franjo Weissing and anonymous referees for comments which greatly improved the manuscript, and are grateful to Nicolas Loeuille for helpful discussion. This work was supported by the French Agence Nationale de la Recherche, grant ANR-06-BLAN-0268.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adam L Cronin.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

Both authors contributed to the manuscript and the development of the model, and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cronin, A.L., Monnin, T. Self-sacrifice in 'desperado' contests between relatives. Front Zool 7, 27 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-7-27

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-7-27

Keywords